An Examination of Factors Related to Suboptimal Cognitive Validity Test Performance in a Mildly Concussed Military Sample

نویسنده

  • Elizabeth M. Seats
چکیده

While cognitive and emotional symptoms which persist beyond what is typical following a mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) are often attributed to the physical or neurological impact of the concussion, numerous studies have suggested that these symptoms are maintained by other factors, such as psychiatric symptoms and compensation seeking (Binder & Rohling, 1996; Feinstein, Ouchterlony, Iverson & Lange, 2003; Somerville & Jardine, 2011). The current study utilized archival data to examine these factors in an active military sample (N=76). Effort and ethnicity’s impact on participants’ cognitive validity test (CVT) performance was also examined. It was hypothesized that a subset of clinical and subscale scores from the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI) would mediate the relationship between ethnicity and performance on CVTs (Test of Memory Malingering and Reliable Digit Span). It was also predicted that involvement with a Medical Evaluation/Physical Evaluation Board would moderate the relationship between PAI results and performance on CVTs. Statistical analyses were run using PROCESS, a logistic regression based program that estimates direct and indirect effects (Hayes, 2012). While some direct effects were found (e.g., higher scores on scales measuring depressive, somatic, and traumatic stress symptoms were associated with failure on a greater number of CVTs), the predicted mediational and moderational relationships were not supported. Results from the study provide support for the notion that cognitive validity tests are associated with psychological and emotional symptoms and not always secondary gain. Additionally, findings show that differences in the way people from different ethnic backgrounds express psychopathology may impact cognitive validity test performance. As not all predictions were supported, future studies AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 3 should examine other factors (e.g., substance abuse) that may influence CVT performance in an active military population with history of mTBI. An Examination of Factors Related to Suboptimal Cognitive Validity Test Performance in a Mildly Concussed Military Sample Since 2001, almost two million U.S. soldiers have deployed to Iraq and Afghanistan in support of Operation Enduring Freedom and Operation Iraqi Freedom/Operation New Dawn (Rigg & Mooney, 2011). It is estimated that as many as 12-35% of U.S. soldiers who have deployed to Iraq and Afghanistan have suffered a mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI; Rigg & Mooney, 2011; Wilk et al., 2010). Mild traumatic brain injuries, or concussions, due to blasts, falls, and blows to the head, have become a much more prevalent issue in U.S. soldiers who have served in the Global War on Terrorism, as compared to previous wars. Whereas blasts would have been fatal in previous wars, better protective equipment has aided in the reduction of fatalities in the wars in Iraq and Afghanistan (Hoge et al., 2008; Warden, 2006; Wilk et al., 2010). The symptoms which commonly follow a mTBI have become a clinical focus in treatment centers for soldiers returning from deployment. The persistence of symptoms, the impact they have on soldiers’ level of functioning, as well as the underlying causes of long-term symptoms are topics of interest to practitioners and researchers alike. Cognitive and physical sequelae commonly associated with mTBI include: difficulties with memory and attention, headaches, and insomnia. Typically the changes in the brain following a mTBI are subtle thus subsequent cognitive impairments are usually temporary and resolve within a short time after the concussion (McCrea, 2008). Continuous problems with recollection and concentration are more commonly seen in individuals who have endured more extensive structural damage to the brain as a result of a severe TBI. Evidence of structural AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 4 changes to the brain, which can result in cognitive difficulties, may be found through the use of neuroimaging tests. While techniques such as CT (Computed Tomography) scans and MRI (Magnetic Resonance Imaging) are useful tools for the detection of structural damage to the brain in emergency neurosurgical situations and severe brain injury, they do not have adequate sensitivity to detect abnormalities characteristic of a typical mTBI (McCrea, 2008). Since neurological changes after a mTBI are usually minor and resolve quickly, researchers have attempted to identify the factors involved in the maintenance of the subsequent cognitive, physical, and emotional symptoms which comprise the disorder known as postconcussional syndrome. Postconcussional Syndrome Postconcussional syndrome (PCS) is a disorder comprised of a collection of symptoms, including impairments in attention, memory, affect, and functional behavior, experienced following a mTBI. This collection of symptoms seen post-mTBI has been recognized for over 100 years, but theories regarding the origin and maintenance of these symptoms have changed significantly over time. Some clinicians have hypothesized that post-mTBI symptoms are the result of organic changes of the brain due to the injury, while others have attributed the symptoms to psychological distress, compensation seeking, or malingering. Strauss and Savitsky (1934) called attention to a small group of clinicians who believed that their patients’ post-mTBI symptoms were psychogenic in nature until they themselves suffered a mTBI and expressed certainty that their symptoms were a result of organic changes in their brains. The controversy centering on the question of whether symptoms and functional impairments, which continue past the expected or usual period of recovery, are maintained by neurological, psychological, or situational factors continues today. Furthermore, there has not AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 5 been one clear, accepted definition of the disorder. The two most commonly referred to definitions of PCS are from the 10 th edition of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD10) and the 4 th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; McCrea, 2008). The criteria of PCS vary somewhat between the ICD-10 and the DSM-IV, however both sources require that a head injury has been sustained (APA, 2000; World Health Organization [WHO], 2008). The ICD-10, a tool used by physicians worldwide to code and classify diseases and symptoms, requires at least three of eight symptoms (i.e., headache, dizziness, fatigue, irritability, insomnia, concentration or memory problems, inability to tolerate distress, emotion, or alcohol) for at least one week following the injury (WHO, 2008). The DSM-5 is the current version of the standardized classification of mental disorders used by mental health practitioners (i.e., counselors, social workers, psychologists, psychiatrists, etc.) in a variety of clinical settings (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The research cited here utilizes the DSM-IV-TR criteria, however. PCS is recognized by the DSM-IV-TR as impairment in cognitive functioning following a closed head injury, with subsequent physical and emotional symptoms that last for at least three months (APA, 2000; Iverson & Lange, 2003). Unlike the ICD-10, which allows for a diagnosis of PCS to be made based on subjective selfreports of impaired cognitive functioning, the DSM-IV-TR criteria for PCS requires that cognitive impairment be verified through neuropsychological assessment (Boake et al., 2004). According to the DSM-IV, in order to receive a diagnosis of PCS, one must experience postconcussive symptoms for at least three months, whereas the ICD-10 requires symptoms for one week post-TBI (APA, 2000; WHO, 2008). Due to the subjective nature in which a diagnosis of PCS is made using the ICD-10, and required time frame (one week versus three months), the AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 6 incidence of PCS is much higher (6 times) than when using the DSM-IV-TR criteria (Boake et al., 2005; McCauley et al., 2005). The most current version of the DSM, the DSM-5, eliminated PCS. Individuals with history of a mild TBI who met DSM-IV criteria for PCS may meet the DSM-5 criteria for Mild Neurocognitive Disorder Due to Traumatic Brain Injury. This diagnosis requires evidence of a TBI with loss of consciousness, posttraumatic amnesia, disorientation and confusion, or neurological signs, as well as evidence (i.e., concern of the individual and modest impairment in cognitive performance) of modest cognitive decline which began immediately following the injury. The DSM-5 states that if the severity of neurocognitive symptoms appear to be inconsistent with the severity of the TBI, somatic symptom disorder may be considered (APA, 2013). Parallel to the DSM-IV criteria for PCS, research showed that symptoms typically resolve within three months following a mTBI (Garden, Sullivan & Lange, 2010; Iverson & Lange, 2003; Meares et al., 2008; Rigg & Mooney, 2011; Vanderploeg, Curtiss & Belanger, 2005). The prevalence of symptoms that persist for more than three months varies across studies with a range of 7 to 36% of individuals (Binder et al., 1997; Boake et al., 2004). For a minority (approximately 5% or less), symptoms continue for six months or more, sometimes for years (Alexander, 1992; Iverson, 2005). Factors associated with PCS. Factors that have been found to be associated with developing PCS, and potentially with the length of time someone reports experiencing PCS symptoms, include injury related characteristics, demographic characteristics (i.e., gender; Meares et al., 2008), IQ (Meares et al., 2008), posttraumatic stress disorder (Meares et al., 2008; Rigg & Mooney, 2011), depression (Iverson & Lange, 2003; Mittenberg & Roberts, 2008), preAN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 7 morbid psychiatric history (Meares et al., 2008; Ponsford et al., 2000), personality traits, litigation status, and expectations about recovery (Mittenberg, DiGiulio, Perrin & Bass, 1992). Although post-concussive symptoms may be, in part, a result of the neurological and physical impact of the concussion itself, many researchers have found evidence that the maintenance of these symptoms is largely attributed to psychological and situational factors (Belanger, Curtiss, Demery, Lebowitz & Vanderploeg, 2005; Binder & Rohling, 1996; Feinstein et al., 2011; Ness, 2010; Paniak et al., 2002; Schneiderman et al., 2008). Identification of the factors that are contributing to the maintenance of post-concussive symptoms is difficult given that many of the symptoms typically seen following a mTBI are also commonly seen in individuals with psychiatric disorders, as well as healthy persons without a history of brain injury (Fox, Lees-Haley, Brown, Williams & English, 1995; Iverson & Lange, 2003; Lees-Hayley & Brown, 1995; Wang, Chan & Deng, 2006). For example, difficulty concentrating, memory problems, fatigue, irritability, persistent depressed mood and/or psychological distress related to health problems, and disordered sleep, all symptoms characteristic of PCS, are also commonly seen in people suffering from chronic pain (SmithSeemiller, Fow, Kant & Franzen, 2003), anxiety states (Horner & Hamner, 2002), and depression (APA, 2000; Iverson & Lange, 2003). It is possible for many of those with PCS that psychological issues or medical conditions were present prior to the mTBI, and thus could account for the cognitive, physical, and/or emotional symptoms they are experiencing. A cry for help is one explanation for an individual’s reporting of persistent PCS symptoms. Relatedly is the idea that people like to be able to label their experiences with a diagnostic label as it provides acknowledgement of and a reason for their negative experiences. Researchers have found that people reported feeling relief and joy upon receiving a diagnosis (Young et al., 2008). AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 8 A study by Iverson and Lange (2003) demonstrated the symptom overlap that exists between PCS and depression, as well as the overlap between PCS and difficulties that exist within non-TBI samples. Healthy community participants, with no history of TBI, completed measures that were designed to assess the presence and severity of depressive symptoms and post-concussive symptoms. The researchers found that the healthy participants reported experiencing a high rate of many of the post-concussive symptoms assessed for (i.e., fatigue, disordered sleep, irritability, temper problems, poor concentration, and memory problems) in this study. Interestingly, based on participants’ endorsement of post-concussive symptoms, 79.6% met DSM-IV symptom (Category C) criteria and 72.1% met ICD-10 symptom criteria for PCS. It was also found that post-concussive symptoms were highly associated with symptoms of depression. Approximately half of individuals who reported at least a mild level of depression reported experiencing a moderate to severe level (based on Likert scales measuring frequency and intensity of symptoms) of PCS symptoms (i.e., nervousness, fatigue, and irritability, sadness, concentration and memory difficulties, and sleep disturbance). Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and PCS are both associated with a set of common difficulties, some of which are included in the symptom criteria for one or both disorders (e.g., difficulty falling or staying asleep, irritability, displays of anger or aggression with little or no provocation, and concentration, attention, and memory deficits; APA, 2000; Horner & Hamner, 2002). Meares and colleagues (2008) examined the relationships between preinjury characteristics, psychological factors, and post-concussive symptoms in a mTBI sample and in a non-brain injured sample, both of which were recruited from a level 1 trauma hospital. Researchers found that 5 days following participants’ injuries, 43% of the mTBI sample and 44% of the non-brain injured sample met ICD-10 criteria for PCS, providing further evidence AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 9 that PCS symptoms are not exclusive to the mTBI population. It was also observed that with increasing levels of PTSD, the likelihood of meeting criteria for PCS became greater. The association between PCS and PTSD was also found in an earlier study that examined the presence of PCS and PTSD symptomatology within samples of mTBI and non-brain injured individuals who were recruited from a trauma hospital following a motor vehicle accident (Bryant & Harvey, 1999). At 6 months post-injury, 20% of the mTBI sample met diagnostic criteria for PTSD, and reported more concentration difficulty, dizziness, fatigue, headaches, sensitivity to noise, and visual disturbance than the mTBI sample who did not meet criteria for PTSD. The authors hypothesized that the intensified anxiety and cognitive load characteristic of PTSD may reduce an individual’s available cognitive resources, thereby contributing to the maintenance of PCS. In other words, PTSD likely complicates the clinical picture and can worsen the impact of mTBI on an individual’s health and functioning (Bryant & Harvey, 1999; Horner & Hamner, 2002; Rigg & Mooney, 2011). Hoge and colleagues (2008) investigated both PTSD and depression as possible mediating variables in the relationship between mTBI and physical health outcomes, positing that these disorders may account for the maintenance of the physical post-concussive symptoms (i.e., stomach and back pain, headache, heart racing, fatigue, sleep disturbance, memory problems, irritability, etc.). The researchers surveyed U.S. soldiers who had recently (within 3 to 4 months) returned from a one-year deployment in Iraq, 15% of whom had sustained a mTBI while deployed. The survey contained questions designed to assess the soldiers’ injuries, combat experiences, physical health/symptoms, cognitive difficulties, symptoms of depression and symptoms of PTSD. Multivariate logistic-regression analysis was used to examine whether PTSD and depression served as mediators between mTBI and physical health outcomes. Hoge AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 10 and colleagues found that the associations between mTBI and physical health outcomes disappeared, except for the association between mTBI and headaches. While headaches were the only physical health outcome associated with mTBI after controlling for PTSD and depression in the analysis, PTSD and depression were each strongly associated with all of the physical health outcomes included in the analyses. The results of the analyses indicated that the physical symptoms experienced in this sample of mTBI soldiers, were mediated largely by psychological difficulties. Assessment of cognitive impairment. Following a mild TBI a neuropsychological evaluation may be completed in order to detect the presence of cognitive impairment and/or disorder, malingering or suboptimal performance due to other factors such as emotional or psychological variables. A neuropsychological evaluation is comprised of information gathering (i.e., social, familial, educational, medical, and psychiatric histories) from self-reports, medical records, and sometimes family members’ reports, as well as a battery of tests which assess major domains of cognitive functioning (i.e., intellectual functioning, academic achievement, language processing, verbal learning and memory, attention and concentration, visual learning and memory, executive functions, processing speed, motor speed and strength, and motivation). Cognitive testing are assessments that quantitatively gauge an individual’s memory, abstract reasoning, verbal fluency, etc. An individual’s scores are compared to the normative data for each assessment. This determines how their performance compares to the average score of the population to which they belong (e.g., age group). Each piece of a neuropsychological evaluation is considered in a clinician’s determination of the individual’s diagnoses and treatment recommendations (Malik, Turner & Sohail, 2013). Psychopathology and Cognitive Validity Testing AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 11 Given the findings that PCS symptoms are experienced by those with and without a mTBI and those diagnosed with other disorders (i.e., PTSD and depression), researchers have questioned what underlying factors are involved in the experience of these symptoms across groups of people with different physical and psychological health histories. Thus a number of researchers have examined the role of psychopathological variables in the experience and report of PCS symptoms (Whiteside et al., 2010). In addition to examining the association between psychiatric symptoms and the experience of PCS symptoms, researchers have also investigated the utility of personality measures as a method of assessing validity (conscious or nonconscious over-reporting) of cognitive symptoms in individuals being evaluated for the presence of a cognitive disorder (Boone & Lu, 1999). Suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators. Suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators is determined by established cutoff scores on various cognitive validity tests. There are a number of explanations for suboptimal performance, including decreased interest and effort due to genuine cognitive impairment or due to a comorbid condition, expectations of failure, stress, and instances of somatization, poor motivation, opposition, secondary gain, and malingering. It is important to emphasize that suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators is not always a case of malingering, which is defined as intentional feigned or exaggerated presentation of symptoms for personal or secondary gain (Strauss, Sherman & Spreen, 2006). Boone and Lu (1999) differentiated between conscious (malingering) and nonconscious symptom production in their study that examined the relationship between a particular Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2; Butcher et al., 2001) profile type and performance on malingering tests. Someone who engages in nonconscious symptom production AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 12 was described by Butcher and colleagues as someone who unintentionally creates symptoms in response to psychological distress; the distress primarily involving somatization/conversion (as indicated by elevations on the Hypochondriasis and Hysteria scales). Elevated somatization/conversion indicates that the individual tends to nonconsciously express his or her psychological distress in the form of physical or neurological symptoms (e.g., pain, headaches, dizziness, vision problems, seizures, etc.). Boone and Lu (1999) stated that discrimination can be made between conscious vs. nonconscious symptom production utilizing scores on the F scale (“faking bad” scale). The sample in their study had an average score of 53.1 on the F scale, suggesting that participants were not over-reporting psychiatric symptoms in a noncredible way. Since the cognitive validity tests used in Boone and Lu’s (1999) study, and in the current study, are traditionally known as malingering tests, they are primarily viewed as methods for detecting conscious symptom exaggeration. Boone and Lu (1999) examined whether or not these malingering tests would also detect nonconscious symptom production of individuals whose highest MMPI-2 elevations were on the Hypochondriasis and Hysteria scales. They retrospectively selected 19 participants, who completed the MMPI-2 and 7 neuropsychological tests from patient files at a Neuropsychology Unit in a major medical center. The researchers found that 13 of the 19 participants exhibited noncredible efforts on the malingering assessments, indicating that symptom production associated with somatization/conversion personality types can stretch to include noncredible cognitive symptoms; not solely physical/neurological complaints. This finding also indicates that test methods designed to detect conscious symptom production, or malingering, may also be able to detect nonconscious symptom production. AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 13 MMPI-2 and PAI. Like Boone and Lu (1999), other researchers have examined the association between elevated scales on the MMPI-2 and performance on cognitive validity indicators. When evaluating the usefulness of a measure in the detection of psychopathology, sensitivity and specificity are two properties examined. Sensitivity is a measure of how well a test is able to detect true positives (e.g., a depression measure has high sensitivity if it is able to detect individuals who are truly depressed). Specificity, on the other hand, is a measure of how well a test recognizes true negatives (healthy individuals without psychopathology). For example, a depression measure with high specificity is able to detect test-takers who do not have depression. Symptom validity tests high in sensitivity and specificity are able to recognize which individuals offered truthful and accurate endorsements of their symptoms and which offered exaggerated or feigned symptom reports, respectively. It has been found that the “conversion V” MMPI-2 profile, which consists of elevations on the Hypochondriasis, Hysteria, and Depression scales, is associated with failure on neuropsychological and cognitive symptom validity tests (Butcher & Williams, 1996; Larrabee, 1998; Smart et al., 2008;). Larrabee (1998) noticed that in MMPI-2 profiles of patients claiming to have suffered a mTBI (no abnormal findings on brain scans) with Hypochondriasis and Hysteria elevations, the F (Infrequency) scale would often not be elevated. The F scale was created to detect random and/or infrequent responding, but an elevation on the F scale, when inconsistency measures are within normal limits, can indicate that an individual is purposefully exaggerating their symptoms to portray themselves as being worse off than they actually are (Butcher, Graham, Ben-Porath, Tellegen & Dahlstrom, 2001). Since the F scale is designed to identify overreporting of psychopathology, and contributes only 1 item to the Hypochondriasis and Hysteria scales, Larrabee (1998) suggested AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 14 that the F scale may be insensitive to somatic symptom fabrication or exaggeration. Instead, Larrabee (1998) suggested that the Fake Bad Scale may be a better method for detecting somatic symptom fabrication or exaggeration. The Fake Bad Scale (FBS) is an experimental scale specifically developed, after the original scales of the MMPI-2, in order to address the lack of detection method of individuals who feign or exaggerate cognitive or physical health problems among compensation seeking or litigating individuals (Lees-Haley, English & Glenn, 1991). The FBS has exhibited sensitivity to inflated cognitive, emotional, and somatic complaints in a number of studies involving participants in personal injury litigation (Larabee, 1998; Slick, Hopp, Strauss & Spellacy, 1996). Larrabee (1998) examined case files of litigating, minor brain injury (i.e., mTBI, neurotoxic exposure, or transient hypoxic encephalopathy) patients who demonstrated evidence of noncredible cognitive symptoms, as measured by a failing score on a symptom validity instrument. All patients included in the study had clinical elevations on the MMPI-2 (T=65) or MMPI (T=70) Hypochondriasis and Hysteria scales. Larrabee (1998) commented that both the MMPI-2 and MMPI groups’ mean scale elevations on the Hypochondriasis and Hysteria scales were higher than those observed in chronic pain and TBI patient studies. Only 3 of the 12 participants in the study had elevated scores on the F scale, whereas 11 of the 12 participants had elevations on the FBS scale. These results support the notion that the FBS scale has better sensitivity for the detection of somatic symptom exaggeration in litigating populations. Miller and Donders (2001) also provided support for the validity of the FBS scale in mTBI samples. When comparing a compensation seeking mTBI group, a non-compensation seeking mTBI group and a non-compensation seeking moderate-to-severe TBI group, they found that the compensation seeking group was twice as likely to have elevated FBS scores, indicative AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 15 of symptom exaggeration. They also found significant correlations between TBIs of mild severity, financial compensation seeking, and elevated FBS scores, suggesting that both mTBI and a compensation seeking status significantly increase the chance that symptom exaggeration will be detected on the MMPI-2. Another important finding from the Miller and Donders (2001) study is that both mTBI groups scored paradoxically higher on all MMPI-2 scales than did a moderate-to-severe TBI group. This paradoxical severity effect on the MMPI-2 has been found in TBI samples across numerous studies (Burke, Imhoff & Kerrigan, 1990; Hoffman, Scott, Emick & Adams, 1999; Leininger, Kreutzer & Hill, 1991; Miller & Donders, 2001; Novack, Daniel & Long, 1984; Youngjohn, Burrows & Erdal, 1995; Youngjohn, Davis & Wolf, 1997). Leininger and colleagues (1991) compared MMPI-2 profiles of mTBI and severe TBI groups and found that while the pattern of scale elevations were very similar between groups (the majority of the mean scale elevations for the two severity groups were separated by 5 or fewer points), the mTBI group scored significantly higher than the severe TBI group on the Hypochondriasis, Hysteria, and Psychasthenia scales. These findings indicate that the mTBI group endorsed more preoccupation with physical symptoms and concentration difficulties than the severe TBI group. The Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI; Morey, 1991) is a 344-item test of personality designed to assess psychopathological constructs relevant to many major mental disorders in adults. The PAI is comprised of validity indices, clinical scales, and clinical subscales. Items on are rated on a 4-point scale from False to Very True. The PAI was developed using a construct validation framework. This framework involved the selection of constructs based on their importance within the mental health classification system and within clinical practice (Morey, 2007). The developer determined the importance of these constructs by AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 16 reviewing historical and contemporary literature. Based on their findings, 18 construct scales were created. Items for each of these scales were developed by reviewing the literature on each specific construct and identifying the components that were most central to the definition of each construct. Questions were then created in order to assess these components (Morey, 2003). The development of the PAI differed fundamentally from the development of the MMPI2. Rather than taking a more theoretical approach to the development of scales, as did the PAI developers, the original MMPI developers used an empirical criterion-keying approach. The emphasis in this approach to test development is the sensitivity (i.e., ability of items to detect the clinical population they are designed to detect) and specificity (i.e., ability of items to discriminate one group of people from another) of items, whereas in the development of the PAI, more weight was put on the actual theoretical content of the items (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013, p.223). In the criterion-keying framework, items were collected from sources like psychiatric case studies and reports, text books, and previously published measures. The MMPI developers then narrowed down the pool of items so that there was not any redundancy. The items were then given to groups of patients who were categorized by their diagnosis and to a “normal”, nonpatient group. Items that discriminated among the normal and the clinical groups were kept. The scales were then cross-validated using different normal and clinical groups (Nichols, 2011). The assessment was updated and restandardized for the MMPI-2 in the early 1980s. The MMPI-2 included new items added to address topics of clinical concern that were not included in the original item collection. The disadvantages of using the MMPI-2 are that, because of the method used in its development, internal consistency reliability is not very high for most of its validity and clinical scales (α= .57 to .74 for validity scales; α= .39 to .87 for clinical scales; Hilsenroth & AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 17 Segal, 2004, p.32). The PAI also holds practical advantages over the MMPI-2, such as shorter administration time, lower required reading level, and lower cost of administration. Measures like the MMPI-2 and PAI are utilized in a number of contexts and for various purposes. For instance, rehabilitation counselors may ask clients to take the MMPI-2 or PAI to assess for job-relevant behaviors and temperament in order to gauge their readiness for employment and to match them up with a job. In a clinical setting, a personality measure may be used to assess for a client’s psychopathological symptoms or experiences, and to assist the clinician in making a diagnosis and/or developing a treatment plan. The current study utilizes the PAI in its examination of whether mood and/or psychopathological experiences, not necessarily psychological disorders, are associated with CVT performance. While evidence supports the adequacy of validity indices on the PAI in the detection of psychopathological symptom exaggeration or malingering, the same sensitivity to the detection of cognitive symptom exaggeration has not been found. The three validity scales developed to identify test-takers who are endorsing items in a way to portray themselves more negatively are the Negative Impression Management (NIM) scale, the Malingering Index (MAL), and the Rogers Discriminant Function (RDF; Morey, 2007). The NIM scale detects profiles that portray someone as having more severe psychopathology (potentially including subjective cognitive impairments in memory and attention) than would normally be reported by an objective observer describing the respondent. If the NIM score is elevated, it may be indicative of malingering. An elevated NIM score may also be indicative of someone who has a more negative style of selfevaluation, which is characteristic of some mental disorders, like depression (Morey, 2003). The MAL, another indicator of negative impression management, is comprised of features that are more often observed in PAI profiles of individuals imitating a mental disorder than in actual AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 18 patients with the disorder. The RDF, a third indicator of negative impression management, was created to discriminate between real patients and individuals simulating the real patients (Morey, 2007). The RDF is a formula, which includes weighted combinations of 20 different PAI scores, and for which a score of greater than 0 indicates malingering (Morey, 2003). Findings from numerous studies indicate that the RDF has the highest sensitivity (.63 to .95) and specificity (.80 to .96) of the three negative impression management scales on the PAI (Morey & Lanier, 1998; Rogers, Sewell, Morey & Ustad, 1996; Scragg, Bor & Mendham, 2000), and large effect sizes when distinguishing authentic patients from simulator patients (Bagby et al., 2002). Morey (2003) explains that the RDF likely exhibits superior sensitivity and specificity because it appears to be free from any influence of true psychopathology, unlike the NIM and MAL scales, and thus detects effortful presentation distortion. This was demonstrated during standardization studies, when the RDF yielded similar mean scores and standard deviations across community (healthy individuals) and clinical (individuals with genuine psychological symptoms/diagnoses) samples (Morey, 2007). The negative profile distortion indicators of the PAI do an adequate job of detecting psychological symptom exaggeration and distinguishing between actual and simulated mental health patients. The little research that has been done has had inconsistent findings regarding PAI validity scales’ ability to detect cognitive symptom exaggeration or malingering. Few of these studies have shown any significant association with failure on cognitive symptom validity testing. The NIM scale was found to have a moderate significant association with the Victoria Symptom Validity Test (Slick, Hopp, Strauss & Thompson, 1997) and the Test of Memory Malingering (Tombaugh, 1996), two instruments designed to assess the validity of cognitive symptom complaints (Haggerty, Frazier, Busch & Naugle, 2007; Whiteside, Dunbar-Mayer & AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 19 Waters, 2009). Armistead-Jehle (2010) examined performance on cognitive validity tests in U.S. veterans referred for a neuropsychological evaluation after sustaining mTBI. He found that there were no significant differences between veterans who passed and veterans who failed the Medical Symptom Validity Test (Green, 2004), a verbal memory measure, on any of the PAI validity scales used (NIM, MAL, and RDF). There is also a lack of research on the relationship between PAI clinical scales and cognitive validity testing. Whiteside et al. (2010) found that in a neuropsychological sample the Somatic Complaints (SOM) scale was the PAI clinical scale that had the most significant and consistent relationship with performance on Trial 2 of the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM), a test that detects noncredible memory dysfunction. The findings suggest that very high SOM scores (T > 87) should raise concerns about the credibility of cognitive symptoms. It was also found that the Anxiety, Schizophrenia, and Depression clinical scales trended toward a significant association with Trial 2 of the TOMM. Additionally, when examining PAI clinical subscales, researchers found a significant association between SOM-C (cognitive) and Trial 2 of the TOMM, however significant associations between the other SOM subscales and Trial 2 of the TOMM were not found. To the author’s knowledge there have not been any other studies that have examined the relationship between PAI clinical subscales and cognitive validity tests. It is important to note that particular PAI scales have been found to be elevated in some mTBI samples. Demakis and colleagues (2007) conducted a study in which they examined PAI data from mTBI and moderate to severe TBI patients located in either a rehabilitation or military hospital. The study participants consisted of 60 civilians and 35 members of the armed forces. The researchers found that there were no significant differences on PAI clinical scale scores between the two groups with different TBI severity levels. Most participants (65%) had elevated AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 20 clinical scale scores, the highest being on the Somatic Complaints, Depression, Borderline Features, Paranoia, and Schizophrenia scales. These elevations on the PAI are similar to what has been found previously in the TBI population using the MMPI-2, which were elevations on the Hypochondriasis, Depression, Schizophrenia, and Hysteria scales (Alfano et al., 1992; Butcher & Williams, 1996; Hessen & Nestvold, 2009; Kurtz, Shealy & Putnam, 2007; Leininger, Kreutzer & Hill, 1991). Demakis and colleagues (2007) were not aware of the litigation status of their participants and therefore acknowledged that litigation status may have influenced the lack of differences found between severity groups in PAI scores. Till and colleagues (2009) suggested that the reason for specific PAI scale elevations in mTBI samples may be that these scales are comprised of some items which partially reflect the physical/neurological symptoms commonly experienced after sustaining a mTBI. The researchers distinguished between transdiagnostic and nontransdiagnostic items on the PAI. Transdiagnostic items are defined as those items that are associated with their intended content as well as physical and neuropsychological symptoms related to TBI (e.g., “My thinking has become confused” from the SCZ-T subscale and “I’ve been moving more slowly than usual” from the DEP-P subscale). In other words, these items assess symptoms that are found in both psychiatric and neurological disorders. Till et al. defined nontransdiagnostic items as items which reflect behavioral disorders common in individuals with a TBI, but which do not appear to be confounded with neurological symptoms (e.g., “People have to earn my trust” from the PARH subscale and “I’ve never been in trouble with the law” from the ANT-A subscale). Transdiagnostic items were identified on the MMPI-2 within a TBI sample, and it was found that when a neurocorrective approach was used to adjust for these items, elevations on the Psychasthenia and Schizophrenia scales significantly decreased (Van Balen, de Mey, & van AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 21 Limbeek, 1999). Elevations on these MMPI-2 scales reflect difficulty concentrating and disorganized thinking, as well as more affective symptoms. The decrease in these scales after correction demonstrates that elevations on these scales within a TBI sample were due to the neurological and cognitive post-TBI symptoms as opposed to symptom exaggeration or symptoms of schizophrenia. There has been little research exploring transdiagnostic items vs. nontransdiagnostic items on the PAI. One published study conducted by Till and colleagues (2009) found transdiagnostic items between psychological and neurological symptoms within the Depression (DEP), Somatic Complaints (SOM), and Schizophrenia (SCZ) scales of the PAI. They found, as well, that participants who demonstrated better functional independence in both cognitive and motor areas were less likely to endorse these transdiagnostic items. This suggests that these items are associated with mTBI sequelae and are more likely to be endorsed by individuals with a history of mTBI, thereby potentially elevating the scales to which they belong. When Till et al. corrected the DEP, SOM, and SCZ scales for the transdiagnostic items within each, there were significant changes in the scores for the SOM and SCZ scales. The change in the SOM scale score is congruent with what Morey (2007) states in the PAI manual that elevations on the SOM scale may reflect functional and/or organic symptoms. The significant change seen on the SCZ scale after correction supports Morey’s assertion that elevations on the SCZ-T (thought disorders) subscale may indicate cognitive symptoms caused by something other than schizophrenia (Morey, 2003). Till et al. (2009) advised that, because of these significant changes after correction, the SOM and SCZ scales be interpreted with caution within a TBI population. In other words, elevations on these scales may not necessarily be indicative of somatization or AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 22 psychological symptoms, rather they may be due to genuine cognitive dysfunction or feigned symptoms for secondary gain. Most of the research examining psychopathology in mTBI populations has utilized the MMPI-2. A few investigators have been examining the utility of the PAI in identifying participants demonstrating suboptimal effort in cognitive testing. For example, Kurtz et al. (2007) sought out to determine whether the paradoxical effect found with the MMPI-2 and TBI was also demonstrated with the PAI. Participants were selected from an archival database comprised of mild TBI and moderate to severe TBI patients who were seen at one of two rehabilitation hospitals or a private practice. Participants had completed administrations of both the MMPI-2 and the PAI. Kurtz and colleagues found that the mTBI group scored higher than the moderate to severe TBI group on all of the 10 basic scales of the MMPI-2, and significantly so on the Hypochondriasis, Depression, Hysteria, and Psychasthenia scales. On the PAI, it was found that mean score elevations across the 11 clinical scales were not significantly different between the two groups, with the exceptions of the mTBI group having significantly higher scores on the Somatization and Depression scales, and the moderate to severe TBI group having significantly higher scores on the Antisocial Features and Alcohol Problems scales. These findings partially support the idea that the paradoxical severity effect is specific to the MMPI-2, but also demonstrate that mTBI samples report greater levels of depression and somatization than severe TBI samples on both the MMPI-2 and PAI. The differences in findings between the two measures can partly be explained by the fact that the PAI does not have equivalents to the Hysteria and Psychasthenia scales on the MMPI-2. Kurtz and colleagues (2007) suggested that the paradoxical severity effect was not seen on PAI scales other than Somatization and Depression because the PAI has better discriminant AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 23 validity than the MMPI-2. Specifically, the MMPI-2 at instrument development included items which discriminated between patients who had a particular disorder and non-patients, while the PAI included items, which not only discriminated between patients and non-patients, but also between patients with one disorder and patients with another disorder (Morey, 2007). Since many symptoms are transdiagnostic, an assessment which does not have high discriminant validity may show high scores on multiple scales for an individual who only has one particular disorder. This may partly explain why mTBI individuals scored significantly higher than moderate or severe TBI individuals on all MMPI-2 clinical scales. Since mTBI is more commonly associated with neurotic and somatic symptoms as compared to more severe TBI, it is reasonable to expect that MMPI-2 scales reflecting these types of symptoms would be more elevated in the mTBI group, however this does not explain higher scores on other MMPI-2 scales. Military and mTBI. In order to know how military personnel affected by mTBI are likely to score on the PAI, it is important to be aware of how military personnel who have not experienced a mTBI score on the PAI. Because the daily life and work of a soldier, particularly when deployed, is vastly different from that of a civilian, their PAI profiles may reflect these differences. Deployed soldiers are at a substantial risk for harm, are separated from their loved ones and often experience or witness potentially traumatic events. While these aspects make deployed military personnel unique, Morey et al. (2011) found that PAI scale scores were similar in a sample of soldiers deployed in Iraq, without a history of TBI, and a sample of the original PAI community standardization sample, matched for age and gender. Morey and colleagues’ (2011) research suggests that deployed military personnel and civilians report similar levels of anxiety, depression, anger, health concerns, stress, suicidal thoughts, and social support. AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 24 Previous data has demonstrated that rates of PTSD in military members who served in Iraq and Afghanistan are three times those found in the civilian population (Schell & Marshall, 2008). The differences in results between the Morey et al. (2011) and Schell and Marshall (2008) studies may stem from differences in study methodology, specifically time between end of deployment and time of evaluation. Morey and colleagues (2011) evaluated soldiers during their deployment in Iraq, while Schell and Marshall (2008) evaluated soldiers as long as 36 months after the end of their last deployment. This is an important difference, because there is data that suggests that rates of PTSD increase with time following the end of a deployment, indicating a “late onset PTSD” (Sundin, Fear, Iverson, Rona & Wessely, 2010). Morey and colleagues discussed one possible interpretation; that soldiers during deployment have adaptive mechanisms that sustain them in a high-stress environment, permitting them to normalize their experiences in a way that may not be maintained in post-deployment life. Fontana and Rosenheck (2008) identified factors, such as social support and youth, that serve as adaptive advantages for soldiers coping with stress or adversity after returning from deployment. Schnurr and colleagues’ (2004) examined factors related to the development and maintenance of PTSD and they found that weakening levels of soldiers’ social and emotional support was associated with the development of PTSD. The combination of results from these studies suggest that depending on the time of evaluation, soldiers may or may not report similar levels of emotional difficulties on the PAI as civilian populations. Ethnicity and the PAI. Differences between ethnic groups in their expression of psychopathology were first noted and studied in the early 20 th century by the famous psychiatrist Emil Kraepelin on his travels from Germany to Asia and North America. According to Marsella and Yamada (2000), the western psychiatry worldview in favor of universal expression of AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 25 psychopathology remains the predominant perspective and has been challenged more in recent years as ethnic minorities and members of non-Western cultures have grown in number and in influence among the mental health sciences. Despite the increased focus on cultural factors and mental health, there has been little research examining the role of ethnicity in the assessment of psychopathology. One of the few studies conducted found a medium effect size in the relationship between African Americans and the tendency to report distress in terms of physical symptoms (Cuellar & Paniagua, 2000). This finding was also reflected on MMPI-2 scores when Castro and colleagues (2008) examined MMPI-2 clinical (CS) and restructured clinical (RCS) scales in a sample of Caucasian and African American outpatient therapy clients. They found that African American participants scored significantly higher than Caucasian participants on the Hypochondriasis (CS1) and Somatic Complaints (RC1) scales, in addition to a few other scales (i.e., Cynicism [RC3], Ideas of Persecution [RC6], and Aberrant Experiences [RC8] scales). Consistent with Castro et al’s (2008) findings, an earlier investigation found that African Americans were more likely than Caucasians to meet full criteria for Somatization Disorder (Zhang & Snowden, 1999). Latino individuals also tend to express emotional distress as physical symptoms (Cuellar & Paniagua, 2000). It has been reported that somatic symptoms are more commonly seen in individuals of lower level socioeconomic status (Cuellar & Paniagua, 2000). Cueller & Paniagua (2000) and Choudhury (2003) suggest that this may partly explain why Hispanics and African Americans are more likely than Caucasians to express distress in physical terms, as disparities in income and educational level exist between ethnic groups. While not extensive, there has been some research conducted which examined ethnic differences in symptom reporting post-mTBI or within a military sample. Over 1,300 Hispanic, AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 26 African American, and Caucasian TBI patients (50.3% severe, 16.3% moderate, and 33.3% mild) from the National Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation’s TBI Model Systems (TBIMS) database were compared on their endorsement of post-TBI symptoms (Arango-Lasprilla, 2012). Participants completed the Neurobehavioral Functioning Inventory (NFI; Marwitz, 2000), a selfreport measure comprised of six subscales (depression, somatic, memory/attention, communication, aggression, and motor), which assesses presence and severity of symptoms common to TBI. Before adjusting for injury demographics, it was found that Hispanic participants displayed significantly higher levels of somatic complaints, communication deficits, and motor impairment compared to Caucasian participants, as well as significantly higher levels of depression, memory/attention difficulties, and aggressive behaviors compared to Caucasian and African American participants. When injury demographics (i.e., Functional Independence Measure, Disability Rating Scale, and length of stay in acute care or rehabilitation; injury severity was not included in adjustment due to the unavailability of Glasgow Coma Scale scores for many of the participants) were accounted for, it was found that Hispanics scored significantly higher on the depression, somatic difficulties, and memory/attention difficulties subscales compared to Caucasians and African Americans. Additionally, the Hispanic participants had significantly higher scores on the communication deficits and motor impairment compared to Caucasians. Notably, there were no differences between Caucasians and African Americans on any of the NFI subscales after adjusting for injury characteristics (Arango-Lasprilla, 2012). Some studies have also found that Hispanics report more symptoms of depression than do Caucasians and African Americans following a TBI, however other studies have found similar levels of depression among African Americans and Hispanics following a TBI (Dunlop, Song, Lyons, Manheim & Chang, 2003; Seel et al., 2003). These mixed findings may partly be due to AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 27 methodology differences. For example, Arango-Lasprilla (2012), who found higher depressive symptom reporting in Hispanics than African Americans, measured depression one year postTBI, while Seel (2003), who found comparable levels of depression between Hispanics and African Americans, measured depression almost three years post-TBI. Additionally, ArangoLasprilla (2012) and Seel (2003) used different operational definitions of depression in their studies, which may have contributed to the differences found in levels of depression across ethnic groups. Arango-Lasprilla (2012) operationally defined depression as the score on the Depression subscale of the NFI, whereas Seel (2003) grouped NFI items together within each of the nine depressive symptom domains described in the DSM-IV-TR and then organized the domains into three major categories (mood, somatic, and cognitive). Items in the Seel (2003) study were categorized as clinically problematic or clinically non-problematic depending on how they were rated by participants on a 5-point scale. Mixed findings also exist regarding ethnic differences in psychopathology within veteran populations. For example, Frueh and colleagues (1997) compared Caucasian and African American veterans who were being treated at a VA outpatient PTSD clinic on measures of depression and PTSD and on the MMPI-2. They found no significant differences between groups on any of the measures they examined. Munley et al. (2001) examined ethnic differences on the MMPI-2 in African American and Caucasian male inpatient psychiatric patients at a VA medical center who were matched in terms of primary discharge diagnosis and marital status. Unlike Frueh and colleagues, Munley et al. also assessed for possible differences on the content scales of the MMPI-2. Consistent with Frueh and colleagues, Munley et al. found no significant differences between Caucasians and African Americans on the validity, clinical, or supplementary MMPI-2 scales. On four of the content scales (Fears, Bizarre Mentation, AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 28 Cynicism, and Antisocial Practices), however, African Americans scored significantly higher than Caucasians. A significant difference between Caucasians and African Americans on the Bizarre Mentation content scale was also found in a 2002 study that examined MMPI-2 profiles in veterans with combat-related PTSD (Monnier, Elhai, Frueh, Sauvageot & Magruder, 2002). However, Monnier and colleagues (2002) found no other significant differences between Caucasians and African Americans on any other MMPI-2 scales. Munley (2001) explained that the difference between ethnic groups found on the Antisocial Practices content scale may be related to the significant differences in the type of substance abuse comorbidity (i.e., a diagnosis of alcohol or drug abuse/dependence in addition to the primary discharge diagnosis) between groups. There was no significant difference in the number of substance abuse comorbidity diagnoses found between ethnic groups in the Monnier (2012) study, however differences in type of substance abuse comorbidity was not reported. Munley (2001) explained that the significant differences found between ethnic groups on the FRS and CYN content scales may be related to differences in worldviews. Perhaps African Americans, because they are more often victims of discrimination and prejudice, tend to have more negative and fixed worldviews. While there have been a number of investigations examining ethnic differences on the MMPI-2, there is a lack of research on ethnic differences on the PAI. The MMPI was first published in 1943 and has been called the gold standard for personality testing (Peltier, 2011). In 1989 the MMPI-2 was published utilizing a new normative data set more representative of the current population characteristics. The PAI is comprised of clinical scales and subscales, some which measure similar psychopathological symptoms as scales on the MMPI-2 (e.g., somatic complaints, depression, and bizarre mentation). Scores on MMPI-2 scales measuring these psychopathological constructs were found to be different between ethnic groups. The SOM AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 29 clinical scale on the PAI measures levels of concern about physical health and perceived distress related to physical symptoms. The DEP scale measures the overall level of depression, while the DEP subscales measure the physiological, cognitive, and affective components of depression (Morey, 2003). While there is no PAI scale that measures all of the symptoms that the MMPI-2 grouped together to call ‘bizarre mentation’, the PAR-P subscale of the PAI measures persecution paranoia, a major feature of the BIZ MMPI-2 content scale. The current study will examine differences between ethnic groups on the PAI clinical scales and subscales of interest, as well as examine whether or not concern about or distress related to physical symptoms acts as a partial mediator in the relationship between ethnic minority status and suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators. Medical/Physical Board Evaluation The MEB and PEB are part of a process utilized to determine if a soldier with a medical illness, including a mental health condition(s) or physical injury, is fit to continue active duty in the military. Medical and Physical Evaluation Boards (MEB/PEB) are panels composed of administrative officers and active duty physicians. A MEB is initiated if an active duty military member has physical or mental health problems that are incompatible with continuing military duty, or if they have been prohibited to deploy overseas for 12 months due to their medical problems (Powers, 2007). The MEB is comprised of active duty physicians, who review clinical case files of individuals deemed unhealthy or injured. Using an established list of medical standards for continued military service, they make a decision about whether the individual has a medical condition that is incompatible with continued military service. If the MEB determines that an individual may not be fit to return to duty, the case then gets passed on to the PEB, a board who reviews the case and makes a recommendation that the individual returns to duty AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 30 (with or without assignment limitations), be placed on a temporarily disabled list, be separated from active duty, or be medically retired from the military. The PEB’s recommendation is given to a central medical board, where the ultimate decision is made. If the decision is made to medically retire or separate a soldier from active duty, he or she may receive retirement payments if they are eligible. Eligibility is based on the number of years a soldier has served in active duty and on the reason the soldier being retired (Powers, 2007). Since the MEB and/or PEB process can potentially result in receiving monetary compensation, similar to litigation due to an injury, involvement in the MEB/PEB process can result in secondary gain. The process of going before the Medical and/or Physical Evaluation Board (MEB/PEB) is one factor that may impact a soldier’s performance on neuropsychological and personality assessments. Findings have been mixed when it comes to the question of whether or not litigation/compensation seeking has an effect on symptom reporting. A study by Lees-Haley and Brown (1993) examined differences in levels of reported neuropsychological symptoms between a personal injury claimant group (non-TBI patients filing claims for emotional and/or workrelated stress) and a control group (patients recruited from a family practice clinic visiting for symptoms including sore throat, respiratory complaints, headaches, flu, hypertension, etc.). It was found that the personal injury claimants reported higher levels of symptoms than did the control group, the highest of which were symptoms of anxiety, disordered sleep, depression, and headaches. One explanation for the difference in symptom reporting is that the personal injury group sought compensation due to greater level of symptoms experienced compared to those who were not seeking compensation. While neither group consisted of head-injured individuals, the compensation-seeking group was found to be significantly more likely to endorse neuropsychological symptoms as compared to the control group (Lees-Haley & Brown, 1993). AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 31 The endorsement of neuropsychological symptoms by non-head injured individuals has been seen across a number of studies (Fox et al., 1995; Hoge et al., 2008; Iverson & Lang, 2003; LeesHaley & Brown, 1993). A number of studies have compared groups of head injured individuals to examine whether an association exists between compensation seeking/litigation status and symptom endorsement. The direction of causality in the association between compensation seeking and symptom severity is complicated. A number of arguments exist, one being that individuals experiencing a greater amount or severity of symptoms would be more likely to become involved in litigation or seek compensation. Another is that individuals who decide to seek compensation purposely report a greater number or severity of symptoms than what they actually experience in order to increase their chance of receiving compensation. Other explanations for the association between compensation seeking and symptom severity include attention seeking and nonconscious symptom production or exaggeration. Since symptoms are frequently assessed for via self-report measures, not all of which have validity indices, it is difficult for clinicians to determine the true number and severity of an individual’s symptoms. Even symptom validity tests, which are able to detect the credibility of the examinee’s symptom reporting, do not have the capability to explain the underlying cause(s) of an examinee’s invalid responses. Malingering or symptom exaggeration is only one explanation for noncredible or suboptimal performance on a symptom validity test. Paniak et al. (2002) found that a compensation seeking mTBI group reported a greater level of symptoms (common complaints of those with a traumatic brain injury) than did the noncompensation seeking mTBI group, even after receiving a treatment rated highly by patients. The only variable found in this study to be positively predictive of the decision to seek compensation AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 32 following a mTBI was a prescription of post-injury analgesic, neurological, or psychopharmacological medications. It would make sense that individuals who experience a greater level of symptoms would be more likely to be prescribed or to take these types of medications; therefore the relationship between post-injury prescriptions and compensation seeking status could be explained by a greater severity of symptoms. To investigate this relationship, Paniak and colleagues (2002) covaried a prescription of analgesic, neurological and/or psychopharmacological medication in a repeated measures analysis. They found that a significant difference in symptom reporting between those who were and were not compensation seeking still existed, indicating that other factors may be related to this difference in groups. Paniak and colleagues suggested that those who seek compensation may report more symptoms or more severe symptoms because they feel the need to convince compensators. This explanation does not discredit the presence of symptoms in those seeking compensation, but instead suggests that greater symptom reporting by compensation seekers is a means of increasing their chance at receiving compensation. Dunn and colleagues’ (1995) findings may offer support to the idea that symptom overreporting and compensation seeking are attributable to pre-morbid emotional/psychological difficulties. Dunn et al. examined the reporting of neuropsychological symptoms in three groups of participants: a non-claimant group with a history of head injury, a non-claimant group without a history of head injury (who were seeking an examination or treatment with their family physician), and a personal claimant group seeking an evaluation for emotional/psychological problems (no history of head injury or exposure to neurotoxins). Researchers found that the personal claimant group seeking compensation for an emotional/psychological evaluation reported suffering from significantly more neurotoxin and/or neuropsychological symptoms than AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 33 did the non-claimant group with a history of head trauma or toxin exposure. While results from this study may be explained by pre-morbid emotional/psychological problems, just as reasonable is the explanation that those who seek compensation feign or exaggerate their symptoms. Researchers have attempted to better understand the relationship between compensation seeking and symptom reporting by utilizing assessments that are not face valid and include validity indices. A study by Youngjohn and colleagues (1997) examined MMPI-2 scores within a group of moderate TBI participants, a group of severe TBI participants (some of whom were involved in litigation) and a group of litigating mTBI participants. They found that severe TBI participants who were litigating had significantly higher elevations on the Hypochondriasis, Hysteria, Schizophrenia, and Health Concerns scales than did the non-litigating severe TBI group. The researchers noted that both the litigating and non-litigating severe TBI groups had equivalent severity indicators (no significant differences in hours of loss of consciousness, Glasgow Coma Scale score, and hours of posttraumatic amnesia between groups), which is an indication that both groups sustained injuries of similar severity levels. They found a paradoxical effect (mentioned earlier) as the litigating mTBI group had significantly higher elevations on the Hysteria, Depression, Psychasthenia, and Hypochondriasis scales compared to both of the litigating and non-litigating moderate and severe TBI groups. Putnam and Millis (1994) reviewed the literature on compensation seeking and symptom reporting, and reported that mTBI patients who reported a high level of symptomatology also typically had psychosocial difficulties that increased the likelihood of illness behavior and pursuit of secondary gain. They also pointed out that illness behavior is not necessarily always conscious. There are possible explanations for unconscious illness behavior, such as growing up in a family that has adopted a tendency to express emotional distress as somatic complaints, or AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 34 one that reinforces its children for playing the sick role. These children can carry on their learned behaviors into adulthood without much or any conscious effort, and therefore be more likely to pursue litigation because they believe they are truly disabled. A study by Binder (1993) examined performance on a forced-choice measure of recognition memory (a measure often used when assessing individuals for possible malingering) in three groups of brain-injured individuals: compensation seeking participants with a history of mTBI, compensation seeking individuals with a brain dysfunction, and non-compensation seeking participants with brain dysfunction. Brain dysfunction was defined as an injury that involved structural damage to the brain as evidenced by brain scans, whereas an mTBI did not show neurologic abnormalities on brain scans. Binder (1993) found that the compensation seeking group with mTBI performed significantly worse on the measure of recognition memory than did the non-compensation seeking group and worse than the compensation seeking group with brain dysfunction. These results are contrary to what would be expected. If severity of structural damage to the brain and compensation seeking status are positively associated with failure on a memory assessment, as is the general consensus, one would expect to find that the compensation seeking brain dysfunction group would perform the worst on a forced-choice measure. Binder’s (1993) findings conflict with this expectation, and suggest that the poor performance by the mTBI group is attributable to factors other than genuine memory impairment. Notably, it was found that 17% of the mTBI compensation seeking group and 3% of the brain dysfunction group seeking compensation obtained below chance scores on the measure, indicating that a likelihood that these test-takers were purposely choosing the incorrect answers. While there has been support for a relationship between compensation seeking and symptom reporting, cognitive effort, and scale elevations on personality measures, there have AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 35 also been a number of studies that found no relationship among these variables. Two studies, in which mTBI participants were assessed for cognitive symptom validity, found no significant association between compensation-seeking status and suboptimal performance on cognitive validity tests (Ross, Putnam & Adams, 2006; Stulemeijer, Andriessen, Brauer, Vos & van der Werf, 2007). Ross and colleagues (2006) suggest that their findings may be the result of coding compensation seeking status as a dichotomous variable (whether or not the participant has an unsettled claim related to their injury; instead of coding compensation based on type of litigation [i.e., personal injury litigation or worker’s compensation, etc.]), therefore limiting its range, and possibly underestimating the true effect of compensation seeking status on cognitive validity indicator performance. While the types of litigation that participants are involved in have been reported in most studies, compensation seeking/litigation has typically been coded as a dichotomous variable for purposes of statistical analyses. This is likely due to there being too few participants in a study to be separated out into categories of litigation type, which if done, decreases the statistical power and increases the likelihood for Type II error. Stulemeijer et al. (2007) attributed their lack of significant findings to the design of their study. They recruited participants from the emergency department at a university medical center and informed them that all data collected was for research purposes only and would not be communicated to anyone involved in their litigation process. Also, some of their participants who demonstrated suboptimal performance on cognitive validity measures were on sick leave and receiving monetary benefits from the state, but were not classified as having a “litigation status”. Had these participants been included in the “litigation status” group, there may have been more of an association found between litigation status and noncredible cognitive symptom reporting. AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 36 No significant association between compensation seeking status and MMPI-2 scale elevation was found in a study by Kurtz and colleagues (2007), in which participants sustained either a mild or moderate to severe TBI. They also found a lack of significant association between compensation seeking status and PAI scale elevations, with the exception of the Malingering (MAL) index. It was found that compensation seeking participants scored marginally higher on the MAL index as compared to non-compensation seeking participants; however this difference was not significant. Current Study While there have been studies which have looked at each of the components involved in the current study (i.e., PAI elevations in a mTBI population, associations between PAI elevations and performance on cognitive validity indicators, relationship between compensation seeking status and cognitive validity test performance, and ethnic differences on measures of psychopathology), to the author’s knowledge no study has examined all of these factors, nor have they all been studied within an active duty military population. Most previous research that has examined PAI elevations within a mTBI sample have focused on the main clinical scales. The current study will investigate clinical scale and subscale elevations within a mTBI sample in hopes of gaining a greater understanding of the relationship between symptom reports and cognitive validity assessment in such populations. Elevations on scales that measure somatic concerns have been found in mTBI samples across multiple studies. In addition, elevations on the SOM and SOM-C scales have been shown to be significantly associated with failure of Trial 2 on the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM; Whiteside et al., 2010), one of the cognitive validity indicators used in the current study. Based on the symptoms assessed by the SOM-S and SOM-H subscales, elevations on AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 37 these scales would be expected in a mTBI sample seeking a neuropsychological evaluation for symptoms experienced after a concussion. An elevated SOM-S (Somatization) subscale reflects complaints about common physical concerns (i.e., headaches, back ache, stomach ache, etc.) and general dissatisfaction with overall health. These complaints are commonly reported by individuals who have sustained a mTBI. An elevation on the SOM-H (Health Concerns) subscale indicates a preoccupation with one’s health and attempts to get better. Those who are motivated by some form of secondary gain may exaggerate the physical symptoms and their subjective impact, reflected in these subscales. While particular elevated PAI scales/subscales (SOM and SOM-C) have shown to be significantly associated with failure on Trial 2 of the TOMM, a trend toward significance was found between elevations on the DEP scale and failure on Trial 2 of the TOMM in a study by Whiteside (2010) using participants with significant emotional disorders or mild head injuries. It has also been found that higher levels of depression have been reported by compensation seeking individuals as compared to non-compensation seeking persons with a moderate to severe TBI (Lees-Haley & Brown, 1993; Youngjohn et al., 1997). While, to the author’s knowledge, elevations on the DEP subscales within mTBI samples have not been examined in previous investigations, based on the symptoms assessed by the DEP subscales, it might be expected that mTBI participants would exhibit elevated DEP subscale scores. Elevations on the DEP-C (Cognitive), DEP-A (Affective), and DEP-P (Physiological) reflect decision-making and concentration difficulties, sadness, anhedonia, and dissatisfaction with one’s current condition, and the physical characteristics of depression (i.e., sleep problems, reduced appetite and energy level), respectively (Morey, 2003). The symptoms reflected in the DEP subscale items are also commonly seen in individuals with PCS. AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 38 There is some evidence to support the idea that those with a high level of depressive symptoms are more likely than the non-depressed to demonstrate specific cognitive impairments (inhibition, inhibition/switching, and category fluency) on cognitive performance testing (Hammar, 2009; Hammar, 2011; Trichard, 1995). These particular cognitive impairments are typically assessed for in a standard neuropsychological battery used when evaluating someone with a mTBI. Due to symptom overlap between PCS and PTSD and the suggestion that the presence of PTSD influences the maintenance of postconcussive symptoms, elevations on measures of trauma-related anxiety might be expected within some mTBI samples (Meares et al., 2008; Rigg & Mooney, 2011). Posttraumatic Stress Disorder has been found to be associated with cognitive impairments, specifically in concentration and attention (Rigg & Mooney, 2011), therefore, it may increase the likelihood of demonstrating suboptimal performance on CVTs. Cognitive difficulties, such as memory, attention, and word finding impairment, are complaints common to PCS and the mTBI population. The SCZ-T (Thought Disorder) subscale reflects these problems when elevated. When there is an elevated SCZ-T subscale without an elevation on the SCZ clinical scale, this may suggest problems related to disorders like brain injuries (Morey, 2003). The current study also examines the role of MEB/PEB status on the relationship between elevated PAI scales and suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators. Previous literature has demonstrated that mTBI individuals tend to perform worse on cognitive validity tests than more severe brain injured participants, regardless of whether they were compensation seeking (Binder, 1993). Based on this finding, it is possible that compensation seeking or AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 39 involvement in the MEB/PEB process could serve as a moderator in the relationship between elevated PAI scales and suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators. Given the literature indicating that some cultures tend to express their emotional distress through physical complaints, the relationship between ethnicity and the main constructs of interest will also be examined in the present study. While there have been some mixed results, the majority of the literature has supported that African American and Hispanic individuals have shown higher rates of somatic complaints and somatization disorder diagnoses compared to Caucasians (Castro et al., 2008; Zhang & Snowden, 1999). This may be related to cultural and social differences between ethnic groups, specifically that Hispanics and African Americans may be more likely to experience and/or express depression in physical and possibly cognitive terms (Cuellar & Paniagua, 2000). In the current study, because the sample consists of mTBI patients who report physical and cognitive complaints, elevated scores on the SOM clinical scale and SOM-C (Conversion) subscale are expected to be seen across ethnic groups. However, because of the overarching trends seen in African American and Hispanic samples, there may still be differences found between ethnic groups on the SOM clinical scale and SOM-C subscale. High rates of depressive symptoms have been found in mTBI populations across numerous studies. Some of the literature has found that Caucasians and Hispanics report higher rates of depressive symptoms or have higher rates of depression diagnoses when compared to African Americans in TBI and in non-TBI samples (e.g., Arango-Lasprilla, 2012; Dunlop et al., 2003). An elevation on the DEP-P indicates difficulties with sleep, energy level, and appetite (Morey, 2003). All of these complaints are commonly reported by those who have sustained a mTBI, as well as by those who experience PCS. In the current study it is expected that scores on AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 40 the DEP clinical scale and subscales will be significantly higher for Caucasian and Hispanic participants than for African American participants. There is limited research on which to base a prediction about ethnic-related differences on the SCZ scale and its subscales, however the SCZ-T (Thought Disorder) subscale is important to examine, since elevations on this subscale, in the absence of an elevation on the SCZ clinical scale, can reflect sequelae (concentration and memory deficits) of a brain injury (Morey, 2003). In the Arango-Lasprilla (2012) study, which examined ethnic-related differences in postconcussive symptoms within a TBI sample, Hispanics reported significantly higher levels of memory and attention difficulties when compared to African Americans and Caucasians. There is no clear understanding of why ethnic differences in the report of cognitive symptoms exist following a TBI, however these differences could be related to how Hispanics express their emotional distress in physical or cognitive symptoms. In Hispanic cultures, an individual is considered strong if he or she is able to control oneself when dealing with a stressful time. Hispanics may be less likely to seek treatment or report emotional symptoms for fear of being perceived as weak (Romero, 2000). Perhaps this could account, in part, for the higher reported rate of memory and attention (rather than psychological symptoms) problems in Hispanics in the Arango-Lasprilla (2012) study. While Caucasians and African Americans have shown higher rates of Schizophrenia diagnoses (Arango-Lasprilla, 2012), the current study’s prediction is made based upon which ethnic group has been shown to experience a higher rate of symptoms associated with elevations on the SCZ-T subscale. Endorsement of SCZ-T subscale items (e.g., “My thinking has become confused” and “My thoughts tend to quickly shift around to different things”) indicates concentration and memory deficits and a decreased ability to think clearly (Morey, 1991). AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 41 Interestingly two studies that examined ethnic differences on MMPI-2 scale scores found that African Americans scored significantly higher than Caucasians on the BIZ content scale (Monnier et al., 2002; Munley et al., 2001). The difference found between ethnic groups on the BIZ scale may be related to experiences of discrimination and oppression suffered by African Americans. As a result, African Americans may be more skeptical and less likely to trust others. The PAR-P (Persecution) subscale on the PAI is thought to be similar to the BIZ scale from the MMPI-2, as they both measure persecutory type paranoia, therefore it might be expected that African Americans score significantly higher than Caucasians on the PAR-P subscale. There have been no studies that the author is aware of involving TBI participants who are litigating and/or compensation seeking, which report ethnic differences on cognitive validity test and/or personality test performance. In fact, most of the literature, which examines test performance within a TBI and/or litigation compensation population, does not report the ethnic breakdown of their participants, as they do with other participant demographics (i.e., age, gender, education level, etc.). The current study will examine ethnic differences in the relationship between PAI scale/subscale elevations and suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators. Additionally, the current study will investigate the role of somatization in the relationship between ethnic minority status and suboptimal cognitive validity indicator performance. Individuals from ethnic groups that tend to experience distress through physical symptoms may be thought to be malingering on neuropsychological tests. Results from this investigation will provide objective test data that may reveal significant associations between the way one expresses distress on the PAI and performance on cognitive validity tests, as well as any ethnic differences in these associations. AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 42 Hypotheses Hypothesis 1. It was predicted that significant associations would be found between elevations on certain PAI scales/subscales (e.g., Somatic Complaints [SOM] clinical scale, SOM-Conversion subscale, SOM-Health Concerns subscale, SOM-Somatization subscale, Anxiety-Related Disorders [ARD] Traumatic Stress subscale, Depression [DEP] clinical scale, DEP-Affective subscale, DEP-Cognitive subscale, DEP-Physiological subscale, and Schizophrenia [SCZ] Thought Disorder subscale) and suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators, coded as the number (0, 1, or 2) of failed CVTs (i.e., TOMM and RDS). Failure on CVTs was determined by a score that falls below the established cutoff scores for each of the CVTs. More specifically, it was hypothesized that the more elevated a participant’s scores were on the scales mentioned above, the more likely they were to demonstrate suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators. Clinical PAI scales were included in the first analysis examining their association with performance on CVTs. For any PAI clinical scales found to be significantly correlated with performance on CVTs, a second analysis of each of their subscales were performed to test for significant relationships between subscales and CVT performance. Figure 1. Hypothesis 1 is represented by the highlighted section of the model. AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 43 Hypothesis 2. Secondly, it was predicted that a status of Medical and/or Physical Board Evaluation would serve as a moderator in the relationship between scores on the predicted PAI scales and subscales and performance on cognitive validity indicators. Specifically, it was hypothesized that a status of MEB and/or PEB would increase the strength of PAI scale elevations’ capacity to predict suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators. Clinical PAI scales were included in the first analysis examining their association MEB/PEB status. For any PAI clinical scales found to be significantly correlated with MEB/PEB status, a second set of analyses of each of their subscales were performed to test for significant relationships between subscales and MEB/PEB status. Figure 2. Hypothesis 2 is represented by the highlighted section of the model. Hypothesis 3. The following predictions were made in regards to ethnic differences on the PAI. It was hypothesized that an ethnic status of African American or Hispanic would be significantly more positively correlated with scores on the SOM clinical scale and SOM-C subscale than an ethnic status of Caucasian. It was predicted that an ethnic status of Caucasian or Hispanic would be significantly more positively correlated with scores on the DEP clinical scale than an ethnic status of African American. It was predicted that an ethnic status of Hispanic would be significantly more positively correlated with scores on the SCZ-T subscale than an AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 44 ethnic status of Caucasian or African American. Additionally, it was hypothesized that an ethnic status of African American would be significantly more positively correlated with scores on the PAR-P subscale than an ethnic status of Caucasian. Clinical PAI scales were included in the first analysis examining their association ethnic status. For any PAI clinical scales found to be significantly correlated ethnic status, a second analysis of each of their subscales was performed to test for significant relationships between subscales and ethnic status. Ethnic status was coded using dummy variables. For example, when analyzing the first predicted difference in the association between ethnic status and PAI scores, African American/Hispanic was coded as 0 and Caucasian was coded as 1. Figure 3. Hypothesis 3 is represented by the highlighted section of the model. Hypothesis 4. It was predicted that an ethnic status of Hispanic or African American would demonstrate a significantly greater association with suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators than an ethnic status of Caucasian. Two separate analyses were performed in order to examine the differences in association between Hispanics and Caucasians on CVT performance and African Americans and Caucasians on CVT performance. AN EXAMINATION OF FACTORS RELATED TO SUBOPTIMAL CVT PERFORMANCE 45 Figure 4. Hypothesis 4 is represented by the highlighted section of the model. Hypothesis 5. It was hypothesized that somatic symptoms, as measured by the SOM scale of the PAI, would partially mediate the relationship between ethnicity (Caucasian, African American, or Hispanic) and suboptimal performance on cognitive validity indicators. Figure 5. Hypothesis 5 is represented by the highlighted section of the model.

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

The King-devick Test: an Indicator of Longer-term Cognitive Effects Post-concussion

The King-Devick test (K-D) was introduced in 2011 as a quick, easy and cost-effective way to screen for a concussion. Relying on differences in visual scanning between a baseline and post-injury examination to discern a concussion, the test has been found to be sensitive to concussions immediately (within minutes) following injury. The aim of this paper was to determine whether the K-D is sensi...

متن کامل

The Effect of Neuropsychological Rehabilitation on the Improvement of Cognitive Function related to Accuracy and Attention in Military Shooters with Moderating Effect of Organism Constraint

Background and Aim: The development of functional promotion methods in military tactics, especially shooting, is essential for the military. The aim of this study was to investigate the effect of neuropsychological rehabilitation on the improvement of cognitive function related to accuracy and attention in military shooters with moderating effect of organism constraint. Methods: The current stu...

متن کامل

Evaluating Cognitive Performance of Executive Control Based on Evidence from Attention Network Test and Event-Related Potential in Individuals with Mild Depressive Disorder and Burnout

Introduction: One of the secondary consequences of burnout is a significant reduction in cognitive functions, the most obvious and important of which is the cognitive function of attention and control, which can play a vital role in efficiency and prevention of errors at work in many occupations. On the other hand, a neurological examination is needed to understand better the relationship betwe...

متن کامل

The Comparison and Relationship between Cognitive and Motor Function in Elderly with and without Falling Experience

The evaluation of cognitive and motor performance can lead to a recognition of the risk factors associated with falling. The aim of this study was to compare cognitive and motor functions in elderly with and without experience of falling. In so doing, 60 elderly with and without falling experience voluntarily participated in this study. Mini Mental examination Test, working memory capacity test...

متن کامل

Testing Older Adults for Signs of Age-Related Cognitive Decline: Clock Drawing Test vs.Mini-Mental State Examination

Objective: Mini-mental state examination, a nonspecific measure of global cognitive function, and the clock drawing test, a very concise and specific measure of cognitive function, are among the most widely used screening tests for cognitive dysfunction. The present research aimed to examine the correspondence between MMSE and CDT scores. Methods: A total of 70 older adults (mean(SD) age=69.70...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2014